XIV
GATHERED BY THE WAY
I. THE TRAINING OF
WILD ANIMALS
I
WAS reminded afresh of how prone we all are to regard the actions of the lower
animals in the light of our own psychology on reading “The Training of wild
Animals,” by Bostock, a well-known animal-trainer. Bostock evidently knows well
the art of training animals, but of the science of it he seems to know very
little. That is, while he is a successful trainer, his notions of animal
psychology are very crude. For instance, on one page he speaks of the lion as
if it were endowed with a fair measure of human intelligence, and had notions,
feelings, and thoughts like our own; on the next page, when he gets down to
real business, he lays bare its utter want of these things. He says a lion born
and bred in captivity is more difficult to train than one caught from the
jungle. Then he gives rein to his fancy. “Such a lion does not fear man; he
knows his own power. He regards man as an inferior, with an attitude of disdain
and silent hauteur.” “He accepts his food as tribute, and his care as homage
due.” “He is aristocratic in his independence.”
“Deep in him — so
deep that he barely realizes its existence — slumbers a desire for freedom and
an unutterable longing for the blue sky and the free air.” When his training is
begun, “he meets it with a reserved majesty and silent indifference, as though
he had a dumb realization of his wrongs.” All this is a very human way of looking
at the matter, and is typical of the way we all — most of us — speak of the
lower animals, defining them to ourselves in terms of our own mentality, but it
leads to false notions about them. We look upon an animal fretting and
struggling in its cage as longing for freedom, picturing to itself the joy of
the open air and the free hills and sky, when the truth of the matter
undoubtedly is that the fluttering bird or restless fox or lion simply feels
discomfort in confinement. Its sufferings are physical, and not mental. Its
instincts lead it to struggle for freedom. It reacts strongly against the
barriers that hold it, and tries in every way to overcome them. Freedom, as an
idea, or a conception of a condition of life, is, of course, beyond its capacity.
Bostock shows how
the animal learns entirely by association, and not at all by the exercise of
thought or reason, and yet a moment later says: “The animal is becoming
amenable to the mastery of man, and in doing so his own reason is being
developed,” which is much like saying that when a man is practicing on the
flying trapeze his wings are being developed. The lion learns slowly through
association — through repeated sense impressions. First a long stick is put
into his cage. If this is destroyed, it is replaced by another, until he gets
used to it and tolerates its presence. Then he is gently rubbed with it at the
hands of his keeper. He gets used to this and comes to like it. Then the stick
is baited with a piece of meat, and in taking the meat the animal gets still
better acquainted with the stick, and so ceases to fear it. When this stage is
reached, the stick is shortened day by day, “until finally it is not much
longer than the hand.” The next step is to let the hand take the place of the
stick in the stroking process. “This is a great step taken, for one of the most
difficult things is to get any wild animal to allow himself to be touched with
the human hand.” After a time a collar with a chain attached is slipped around
the lion’s neck when he is asleep. He is now chained to one end of the cage.
Then a chair is introduced into the cage; whereupon this king of beasts, whose
reason is being developed, and who has such clear notions of inferior and
superior, and who knows his own powers, usually springs for the chair, seeking
to demolish it. His tether prevents his reaching it, and so in time he
tolerates the chair. Then the trainer, after some preliminary feints, walks
into the cage and seats himself in the chair. And so, inch by inch, as it were,
the trainer gets control of the animal and subdues him to his purposes, not by
appealing to his mind, for he has none, but by impressions upon his senses.
“Leopards,
panthers, and jaguars are all trained in much the same manner,” and in putting
them through their tricks one invariable order must be observed: “Each thing
done one day must be done the next day in exactly the same way; there must be
no deviation from the rule.” Now we do not see in this fact the way of a
thinking or reflecting being, but rather the way of a creature governed by
instinct or unthinking intelligence. An animal never learns a trick in the
sense that man learns it, never sees through it or comprehends it, has no image
of it in its mind, and no idea of the relations of the parts of it to one
another; it does it by reason of repetition, as a creek wears its channel, and
probably has no more self-knowledge or self-thought than the creek has. This, I
think, is quite contrary to the popular notion of animal life and mentality,
but it is the conclusion that I, at least, cannot avoid after making a study
of the subject.
II. AN ASTONISHED
PORCUPINE
One
summer, while
three young people and I were spending an afternoon upon a
mountain-top, our
dogs treed a porcupine. At my suggestion the young man climbed the tree
— not a
large one — to shake the animal down. I wished to see what
the dogs would dg
with him, and what the “quill-pig” would do with
the dogs. As the climber
advanced the rodent went higher, till the limb he clung to was no
larger than
one’s wrist. This the young man seized and shook vigorously.
I expected to see
the slow, stupid porcupine drop, but he did not. He only tightened his
hold.
The climber tightened his hold, too, and shook the harder. Still the
bundle of
quills did not come down, and no amount of shaking could bring it down.
Then I
handed a long pole up to the climber, and he tried to punch the animal
down.
This attack in the rear was evidently a surprise; it produced an
impression
different from that of the shaking. The porcupine struck the pole with
his
tail, put up the shield of quills upon his back, and assumed his best
attitude
of defense. Still the pole persisted in its persecution, regardless of
the
quills; evidently the animal was astonished: he had never had an
experience
like this before; he had now met a foe that despised his terrible
quills. Then
he began to back rapidly down the tree in the face of his enemy. The
young
man’s sweetheart stood below, a highly interested spectator.
“Look out, Sam,
he’s coming down!” “Be quick, he
‘s gaining on you!” “Hurry,
Sam!” Sam came as
fast as he could, but he had to look out for his footing, and
his antagonist
did not. Still, he reached the ground first, and his sweetheart
breathed more
easily. It looked as if the porcupine reasoned thus: “My
quills are useless
against a foe so far away; I must come to close quarters with
him.” But, of
course, the stupid creature had no such mental process, and formed no
such
purpose. He had found the tree unsafe, and his instinct now was to get
to the
ground as quickly as possible and take refuge among the rocks. As he
came down
I hit him a slight blow over the nose with a rotten stick, hoping only
to confuse
him a little, but much to my surprise and mortification he
dropped to the
ground and rolled down the hill dead, having succumbed to a blow that a
woodchuck or a coon would hardly nave regarded at all. Thus does the
easy,
passive mode of defense of the porcupine not only dull his wits, but it
makes
frail and brittle the thread of his life. He has had no struggles or
battles to
harden and toughen him.
That blunt nose of
his is as tender as a baby’s, and he is snuffed out by a blow that would hardly
bewilder for a moment any other forest animal, unless it be the skunk, another
sluggish non-combatant of our woodlands. Immunity from foes, from effort, from
struggle is always purchased with a price.
Certain of our
natural history romancers have taken liberties with the porcupine in one
respect: they have shown him made up into a ball and rolling down a hill. One
writer makes him do this in a sportive mood; he rolls down a long hill in the
woods, and at the bottom he is a ragged mass of leaves which his quills have
impaled — an apparition that nearly frightened a rabbit out of its wits. Let
any one who knows the porcupine try to fancy it performing a feat like this!
Another romancer
makes his porcupine roll himself into a ball when attacked by a panther, and
then on a nudge from his enemy roll down a snowy incline into the water. I
believe the little European hedgehog can roll itself up into something like a
ball, but our porcupine does not. I have tried all sorts of tricks with him,
and made all sorts of assaults upon him, at different times, and I have never
yet seen him assume the globular form. It would not be the best form for him to
assume, because it would partly expose his vulnerable under side. The one thing
the porcupine seems bent upon doing at all times is to keep right side up with
care. His attitude of defense is crouching close to the ground, head drawn in
and pressed down, the circular shield of large quills upon his back opened and
extended as far as possible, and the tail stretched back rigid and held close
upon the ground. “Now come on,” he says, “if you want to.” The tail is his
weapon of active defense; with it he strikes upward like lightning, and drives
the quills into whatever they touch. In his chapter called “In Panoply of
Spears,” Mr. Roberts paints the porcupine without taking any liberties with
the creature’s known habits. He portrays one characteristic of the porcupine
very felicitously: “As the porcupine made his resolute way through the woods,
the manner of his going differed from that of all the other kindreds of the
wild. He went not furtively. He had no particular objection to making a noise.
He did not consider it necessary to stop every little while, stiffen himself to
a monument of immobility, cast wary glances about the gloom, and sniff the air
for the taint of enemies. He did not care who knew of his coming, and he did
not greatly care who came. Behind his panoply of biting spears he felt himself
secure, and in that security he moved as if he held in fee the whole green,
shadowy, perilous woodland world.”
III. BIRDS AND
STRINGS
A college professor
writes me as follows:
“Watching this morning a robin attempting to
carry off a string, one end of which was caught in a tree, I was much impressed
by his utter lack of sense. He could not realize that the string was fast, or
that it must be loosened before it could be carried off, and in his efforts to
get it all in his bill he wound it about a neighboring limb. If as little sense
were displayed in using other material for nests, there would be no robins’
nests. It impressed me more than ever with the important part played by
instinct.”
Who ever saw any of
our common birds display any sense or judgment in the handling of strings?
Strings are comparatively a new thing with birds; they are not a natural
product, and as a matter of course birds blunder in handling them. The oriole
uses them the most successfully, often attaching her pensile nest to the branch
by their aid. But she uses them in a blind, childish way, winding them round
and round the branch, often getting them looped over a twig or hopelessly tangled,
and now and then hanging herself with them, as is the case with other birds. I
have seen a sparrow, a cedar-bird, and a robin each hung by a string it was
using in the building of its nest. Last spring, in Spokane, a boy brought me a
desiccated robin, whose feet were held together by a long thread hopelessly
snarled. The boy had found it hanging to a tree.
I have seen in a
bird magazine a photograph of an oriole’s nest that had a string carried around
a branch apparently a foot or more away, and then brought back and the end
woven into the nest. It was given as a sample of a well-guyed nest, the discoverer
no doubt looking upon it as proof of an oriole’s forethought in providing
against winds and storms. I have seen an oriole’s nest with a string carried
around a leaf, and another with a long looped string hanging free. All such
cases simply show that the bird was not master of her material; she bungled;
the trailing string caught over the leaf or branch, and she drew both ends in
and fastened them regardless of what had happened. The incident only shows how
blindly instinct works.
Twice I have seen
cedar-birds, in their quest for nesting-material, trying to carry away the
strings that orioles had attached to branches. According to our sentimental “School
of Nature Study,” the birds should have untied and unsnarled the strings in a
human way, but they did not; they simply tugged at them, bringing their weight
to bear, and tried to fly away with the loose end.
In view of the
ignorance of birds with regard to strings, how can we credit the story told by
one of our popular nature writers of a pair of orioles that deliberately
impaled a piece of cloth upon a thorn in order that it might be held firmly
while they pulled out the threads? When it came loose, they refastened it. The
story is incredible for two reasons: (1) the male oriole does not assist the
female in building the nest; he only furnishes the music; (2) the whole
proceeding implies an amount of reflection and skill in dealing with a new problem
that none of our birds possess. What experience has the race of orioles had
with cloth, that any member of it should know how to unravel it in that way?
The whole idea is absurd.
IV. MIMICRY
To what lengths the
protective resemblance theory is pushed by some of its expounders! Thus, in
the neighborhood of Rio Janeiro there are two species of hawks that closely
resemble each other, but one eats only insects and the other eats birds.
Mr. Wallace thinks
that the bird-eater mimics the insect-eater, so as to deceive the birds, which
are not afraid of the latter. But if the two hawks look alike, would not the
birds come to regard them both as bird-eaters, since one of them does eat
birds? Would they not at once identify the harmless one with their real enemy and
thus fear them both alike? If the latter were newcomers and vastly in the minority,
then the ruse might work for a while. But if there were ten harmless hawks
around to one dangerous one, the former would quickly suffer from the character
of the latter in the estimation of the birds. Birds are instinctively afraid of
all hawk kind.
Wallace thinks it
may be an advantage to cuckoos, a rather feeble class of birds, to resemble the
hawks, but this seems to me far-fetched. True it is, if the sheep could imitate
the wolf, its enemies might keep clear of it. Why, then, has not this
resemblance been brought about? Our cuckoo is a feeble and defenseless bird
also, but it bears no resemblance to the hawk. The same can be said of scores
of other birds.
Many of these close
resemblances among different species of animals are no doubt purely accidental,
or the result of the same law of variation acting under similar conditions. We
have a hummingbird moth that so closely in its form and flight and manner
resembles a hummingbird, that if this resemblance brought it any immunity from
danger it would be set down as a clear case of mimicry. There is such a moth in
England, too, where no hummingbird is found. Why should not Nature repeat
herself in this way? This moth feeds upon the nectar of flowers like the
hummingbird, and why should it not have the hummingbird’s form and manner?
Then there are
accidental resemblances in nature, such as the often-seen resemblance of knots
of trees and of vegetables to the human form, and of a certain fungus to a
part of man’s anatomy. We have a fly that resembles a honey-bee. In my
bee-hunting days I used to call it the “mock honey-bee.” It would come up the
wind on the scent of my bee box and hum about it precisely like a real bee. Of
course it was here before the honey-bee, and has been evolved quite
independently of it. It feeds upon the pollen and nectar of flowers like the
true bee, and is, therefore, of similar form and color. The honey-bee has its
enemies; the toads and tree-frogs feed upon it, and the kingbird captures the
slow drone.
When an edible
butterfly mimics an inedible or noxious one, as is frequently the case in the
tropics, the mimicker is no doubt the gainer.
It makes a big
difference whether the mimicker is seeking to escape from an enemy, or seeking
to deceive its prey. I fail to see how, in the latter case, any disguise of
form or color could be brought about.
Our shrike, at
times, murders little birds and eats out their brains, and it has not the form,
or the color, or the eye of a bird of prey, and thus probably deceives its
victims, but there is no reason to believe that this guise is the result of any
sort of mimicry.
V. THE COLORS OF
FRUITS
Mr. Wallace even
looks upon the nuts as protectively colored, because they are not to be eaten.
But without the agency of the birds and the squirrels, how are the heavy nuts,
such as the chestnut, beechnut, acorn, butternut, and the like, to be scattered?
The blue jay is often busy hours at a time in the fall, planting chestnuts and
acorns, and red squirrels carry butternuts and walnuts far from the parent
trees, and place them in forked limbs and holes for future use. Of course, many
of these fall to the ground and take root. If the protective coloration of the
nuts, then, were effective, it would defeat a purpose which every tree and
shrub and plant has at heart, namely, the scattering of its seed. I notice that
the button-balls on the sycamores are protectively colored also, and certainly
they do not crave concealment. It is true that they hang on the naked trees
till spring, when no concealment is possible. It is also true that the jays and
the crows carry away the chestnuts from the open burrs on the trees where no
color scheme would conceal them. But the squirrels find them upon the ground
even beneath the snow, being guided, no doubt, by the sense of smell.
The hickory nut is
almost white; why does it not seek concealment also? It is just as helpless as
the others, and is just as sweet-meated. It occurs to me that birds can do
nothing with it on account of its thick shell; it needs, therefore, to attract
some four-footed creature that will carry it away from the parent tree, and
this is done by the mice and the squirrels. But if this is the reason of its
whiteness, there is the dusky butternut and the black walnut, both more or less
concealed by their color, and yet having the same need of some creature to
scatter them.
The seeds of the
maple, and of the ash and the linden, are obscurely colored, and they are
winged; hence they do not need the aid of any creature in their dissemination.
To say that this is the reason of their dull, unattractive tints would be an
explanation on a par with much that one hears about the significance of animal
and vegetable coloration. Why is corn so bright colored, and wheat and barley
so dull, and rice so white? No doubt there is a reason in each case, but I
doubt if that reason has any relation to the surrounding animal life.
The new Botany
teaches that the flowers have color and perfume to attract the insects to aid
in their fertilization — a need so paramount with all plants, because plants
that are fertilized by aid of the wind have very inconspicuous flowers. Is it
equally true that the high color of most fruits is to attract some hungry
creature to come and eat them and thus scatter the seeds? From the dwarf
cornel, or bunch-berry, in the woods, to the red thorn in the fields, every
fruit-bearing plant and shrub and tree seems to advertise itself to the
passer-by in its bright hues. Apparently there is no other use to the plant of
the fleshy pericarp than to serve as a bait or wage for some animal to come and
sow its seed. Why, then, should it not take on these alluring colors to help
along this end? And yet there comes the thought, may not this scarlet and gold
of the berries and tree fruits be the inevitable result of the chemistry of
ripening, as it is with the autumn foliage? What benefit to the tree, directly
or indirectly, is all this wealth of color of the autumn? Many of the
toadstools are highly colored also; how do they profit by it? Many of the
shells upon the beach are very showy; to what end? The cherry-birds find the
pale ox-hearts as readily as they do the brilliant Murillos, and the dull blue
cedar berries and the duller drupes of the lotus are not concealed from them
nor from the robins. But it is true that the greenish white grapes in the
vineyard do not suffer from the attacks of the birds as do the blue and red
ones. The reason probably is that the birds regard them as unripe. The white
grape is quite recent, and the birds have not yet “caught on.”
Poisonous fruits
are also highly colored; to what end? In Bermuda I saw on low bushes great
masses of what they called “pigeon-berries” of a brilliant yellow color and
very tempting, yet I was assured they were poisonous. It would be interesting
to know if anything eats the red berries of our wild turnip or arum. I doubt if
any bird or beast could stand them. Wherefore, then, are they so brightly
colored? I am also equally curious to know if anything eats the fruit of the
red and white baneberry and the blue cohosh.
The seeds of some
wild fruit, such as the climbing bitter-sweet, are so soft that it seems impossible
they should pass through the gizzard of a bird and not be destroyed.
The fruit of the
sumac comes the nearest to being a cheat of anything I know of in nature — a
collection of seeds covered with a flannel coat with just a perceptible acid
taste, and all highly colored. Unless the seed itself is digested, what is
there to tempt the bird to devour it, or to reward it for so doing?
In the tropics one
sees fruits that do not become bright colored on ripening, such as the
breadfruit, the custard apple, the naseberry, the mango. And tropical foliage
never colors up as does the foliage of northern trees.
VI. INSTINCT
Many false notions
seem to be current in the popular mind about instinct. Apparently, some of our
writers on natural history themes would like to discard the word entirely. Now
instinct is not opposed to intelligence; it is intelligence of the unlearned,
unconscious kind, — the intelligence innate in nature. We use the word to
distinguish a gift or faculty which animals possess, and which is independent
of instruction and experience, from the mental equipment of man which depends
mainly upon instruction and experience. A man has to be taught to do that which
the lower animals do from nature. Hence the animals do not progress in
knowledge, while man’s progress is almost limitless. A man is an animal born
again into a higher spiritual plane. He has lost or shed many of his animal
instincts in the process, but he has gained the capacity for great and
wonderful improvement.
Instinct is opposed
to reason, to reflection, to thought, — to that kind of intelligence which
knows and takes cognizance of itself. Instinct is that lower form of
intelligence which acts through the senses, — sense perception, sense
association, sense memory, — which we share with the animals, though their eyes
and ears and noses are often quicker and keener than ours. Hence the animals
know only the present, visible, objective world, while man through his gift of
reason and thought knows the inward world of ideas and ideal relations.
An animal for the
most part knows all that it is necessary for it to know as soon as it reaches
maturity; what it learns beyond that, what it learns at the hands of the
animal-trainer, for instance, it learns slowly, through a long repetition of
the process of trial and failure. Man also achieves many things through
practice alone, or through the same process of trial and failure. Much of his
manual skill comes in this way, but he learns certain things through the
exercise of his reason; he sees how
the thing is done, and the relation of the elements of the problem to one
another. The trained animal never sees how the thing is done, it simply does it
automatically, because certain sense impressions have been stamped upon it
till a habit has been formed, just as a man will often wind his watch before
going to bed, or do some other accustomed act, without thinking of it.
The bird builds her
nest and builds it intelligently, that is, she adapts means to an end; but
there is no reason to suppose that she thinks
about it in the sense that man does when he builds his house. The nest-building
instinct is stimulated into activity by outward conditions of place and climate
and food supply as truly as the growth of a plant is thus stimulated.
As I look upon the
matter, the most wonderful and ingenious nests in the world, as those of the
weaver-birds and orioles, show no more independent self-directed and
self-originated thought than does the rude nest of the pigeon or the cuckoo.
They evince a higher grade of intelligent instinct, and that is all. Both are
equally the result of natural promptings, and not of acquired skill, or the
lack of it. One species of bird will occasionally learn the song of another
species, but the song impulse must be there to begin with, and this must be
stimulated in the right way at the right time. A caged English sparrow has
been known to learn the song of the canary caged with or near it, but the
sparrow certainly inherits the song impulse. One has proof of this when he hears
a company of these sparrows sitting in a tree in spring chattering and chirping
in unison, and almost reaching an utterance that is song-like. Our cedar-bird
does not seem to have the song impulse, and I doubt if it could ever be taught
to sing. In like manner our ruffed grouse has but feeble vocal powers, and I do
not suppose it would learn to crow or cackle if brought up in the barn-yard. It
expresses its joy at the return of spring and the mating season in its drum, as
do the woodpeckers.
The recent English
writer Richard Kearton says there is “no such dead level of unreasoning
instinct” in the animal world as is popularly supposed, and he seems to base
the remark upon the fact that he found certain of the cavities or holes in a
hay-rick where sparrows roosted lined with feathers, while others were not
lined. Such departures from a level line of habit as this are common enough
among all creatures. Instinct is not something as rigid as cast iron; it does
not invariably act like a machine, always the same. The animal is something
alive, and is subject to the law of variation. Instinct may act more strongly
in one kind than in another, just as reason may act more strongly in one man
than in another, or as one animal may have greater speed or courage than another
of the same species. It would be hard to find two live creatures, very far up
in the scale, exactly alike. A thrush may use much mud in the construction of
its nest, or it may use little or none at all; the oriole may weave strings
into its nest, or it may use only dry grasses and horse-hairs; such cases only
show variations in the action of instinct. But if an oriole should build a nest
like a robin, or a robin build like a cliff swallow, that would be a departure
from instinct to take note of.
Some birds show a
much higher degree of variability than others; some species vary much in song,
others in nesting and in feeding habits. I have never noticed much variation in
the songs of robins, but in their nesting-habits they vary constantly. Thus one
nest will be almost destitute of mud, while another will be composed almost
mainly of mud; one will have a large mass of dry grass and weeds as its
foundation, while the next one will have little or no foundation of the kind.
The sites chosen vary still more, ranging from the ground all the way to the
tops of trees. I have seen a robin’s nest built in the centre of a small box
that held a clump of ferns, which stood by the roadside on the top of a low
post near a house, and without cover or shield of any sort. The robin had
welded her nest so completely to the soil in the box that the whole could be
lifted by the rim of the nest. She had given a very pretty and unique effect to
the nest by a border of fine dark rootlets skillfully woven together. The song sparrow
shows a high degree of variability both in its song and in its nesting-habits,
each bird having several songs of its own, while one may nest upon the ground
and another in a low bush, or in the vines on the side of your house. The
vesper sparrow, on the other hand, shows a much lower degree of variability,
the individuals rarely differing in their songs, while all the nests I have
ever found of this sparrow were in open grassy fields upon the ground. The
chipping or social sparrow is usually very constant in its song and its
nesting-habits, and yet one season a chippy built her nest in an old robin’s,
nest in the vines on my porch. It was a very pretty instance of adaptation on
the part of the little bird. Another chippy that I knew had an original song,
one that resembled the sound of a small tin whistle. The bush sparrow, too, is
pretty constant in choosing a bush in which to place its nest, yet I once found
the nest of this sparrow upon the ground in an open field with suitable bushes
within a few yards of it. The woodpeckers, the jays, the cuckoos, the pewees,
the warblers, and other wood birds show only a low degree of variability in
song, feeding, and nesting habits.
The Baltimore
oriole makes free use of strings in its nest-building, and the songs of
different birds of this species vary greatly, while the orchard oriole makes no
use of strings, so far as I have observed, and its song is always and
everywhere the same. Hence we may say that the lives of some birds run much
more in ruts than do those of others; they show less plasticity of instinct,
and are perhaps for that reason less near the state of free intelligence.
Organic life in all
its forms is flexible; instinct is flexible; the habits of all the animals
change more or less with changed conditions, but the range of the fluctuations
in the lives of the wild creatures is very limited, and is always determined by
surrounding circumstances, and not by individual volition, as it so often is in
the case of man. In a treeless country birds that sing on the perch elsewhere
will sing on the wing. The black bear in the Southern States “holes up” for a
much shorter period than in Canada or the Rockies. Why is the spruce grouse so
stupid compared with most other species? Why is the Canada jay so tame and
familiar about your camp in the northern woods or in the Rockies, and the other
jays so wary? Such variations, of course, have their natural explanation,
whatever it may be. In New Zealand there is a parrot, the kea, that once lived
upon honey and fruit, but that now lives upon the sheep, tearing its way down
to the kidney fat.
This is a wide
departure in instinct, but it is not to be read as a development of reason in
its place. It is a modified instinct, — the instinct for food seeking new
sources of supply. Exactly how it came about would be interesting to know. Our
oriole is an insectivorous bird, but in some localities it is very destructive
in the August vineyards. It does not become a fruit-eater like the robin, but a
juice-sucker; it punctures the grapes for their unfermented wine. Here, again,
we have a case of modified and adaptive instinct. All animals are more or less
adaptive, and avail themselves of new sources of food supply. When the southern
savannas were planted with rice, the bobolinks soon found that this food suited
them. A few years ago we had a great visitation in the Hudson River Valley of
crossbills from the north. They lingered till the fruit of the peach orchards
had set, when they discovered that here was a new source of food supply, and
they became very destructive to the promised crop by deftly cutting out the
embryo peaches. All such cases show how plastic and adaptive instinct is, at
least in relation to food supplies. Let me again say that instinct is native,
untaught intelligence, directed outward, but never inward as in man.
VII. THE ROBIN
Probably, with us,
no other bird is so closely associated with country life as the robin; most of
the time pleasantly, but for a brief season, during cherry time, unpleasantly.
His life touches or mingles with ours at many points — in the dooryard, in the
garden, in the orchard, along the road, in the groves, in the woods. He is
everywhere except in the depths of the primitive forests, and he is always very
much at home. He does not hang timidly upon the skirts of our rural life,
like, say, the thrasher or the chewink; he plunges in boldly and takes his
chances, and his share, and often more than his share, of whatever is going.
What vigor, what cheer, how persistent, how prolific, how adaptive;
pugnacious, but cheery, pilfering, but companionable!
When one first sees
his ruddy breast upon the lawn in spring, or his pert form outlined against a
patch of lingering snow in the brown fields, or hears his simple carol from the
top of a leafless tree at sundown, what a vernal thrill it gives one! What a
train of pleasant associations is quickened into life!
What pictures he
makes upon the lawn! What attitudes he strikes! See him seize a worm and yank
it from its burrow!
I recently observed
a robin boring for grubs in a country dooryard. It is a common enough sight to
witness one seize an angle-worm and drag it from its burrow in the turf, but I
am not sure that I ever before saw one drill for grubs and bring the big white
morsel to the surface. The robin I am speaking of had a nest of young in a
maple near by, and she worked the neighborhood very industriously for food. She
would run along over the short grass after the manner of robins, stopping every
few feet, her form stiff and erect. Now and then she would suddenly bend her
head toward the ground and bring eye or ear for a moment to bear intently upon
it. Then she would spring to boring the turf vigorously with her bill, changing
her attitude at each stroke, alert and watchful, throwing up the grass roots
and little jets of soil, stabbing deeper and deeper, growing every moment more
and more excited, till finally a fat grub was seized and brought forth. Time
after time, during several days, I saw her mine for grubs in this way and drag
them forth. How did she know where to drill? The insect was in every case an
inch below the surface. Did she hear it gnawing the roots of the grasses, or
did she see a movement in the turf beneath which the grub was at work? I know
not. I only know that she struck her game unerringly each time. Only twice did
I see her make a few thrusts and then desist, as if she had been for the moment
deceived.
How pugnacious the
robin is! With what spunk and spirit he defends himself against his enemies!
Every spring I see the robins mobbing the blue jays that go sneaking through
the trees looking for eggs. The crow blackbirds nest in my evergreens, and
there is perpetual war between them and the robins.
The blackbirds
devour the robins’ eggs, and the robins never cease to utter their protest,
often backing it up with blows. I saw two robins attack a young blackbird in
the air, and they tweaked out his feathers at a lively rate.
One spring a pack
of robins killed a cuckoo near me that they found robbing a nest. I did not
witness the killing, but I have cross-questioned a number of people who did see
it, and I am convinced of the fact. They set upon him when he was on the
robin’s nest, and left him so bruised and helpless beneath it that he soon
died. It was the first intimation I had ever had that the cuckoo devoured the
eggs of other birds.
Two other
well-authenticated cases have come to my knowledge of robins killing cuckoos
(the black-billed) in May. The robin knows its enemies, and it is quite
certain, I think, that the cuckoo is one of them.
What a hustler the
robin is! No wonder he gets on in the world. He is early, he is handy, he is
adaptive, he is tenacious. Before the leaves are out in April the female
begins her nest, concealing it as much as she can in a tree-crotch, or placing
it under a shed or porch, or even under an overhanging bank upon the ground.
One spring a robin built her nest upon the ladder that was hung up beneath the
eaves of the wagon-shed. Having occasion to use the ladder, we placed the nest
on a box that stood beneath it. The robin was disturbed at first, but soon went
on with her incubating in the new and more exposed position. The same spring
one built her nest upon a beam in a half-finished fruit house, going out and in
through the unshingled roof. One day, just as the eggs were hatched, we
completed the roof, and kept up a hammering about the place till near night;
the mother robin scolded a good deal, but she did not desert her young, and
soon found her way in and out the door.
If a robin makes up
her mind to build upon your porch, and you make up your mind that you do not
want her there, there is likely to be considerable trouble on both sides before
the matter is settled. The robin gets the start of you in the morning, and has
her heap of dry grass and straws in place before the jealous broom is stirring,
and she persists after you have cleaned out her rubbish half a dozen times.
Before you have discouraged her, you may have to shunt her off of every plate
or other “coign of vantage” with boards or shingles. A strenuous bird indeed,
and a hustler.
VIII. THE CROW
One very cold
winter’s morning, after a fall of nearly two feet of snow, as I came out of my
door three crows were perched in an apple tree but a few rods away. One of them
uttered a peculiar caw as they saw me, but they did not fly away. It was not
the usual high-keyed note of alarm. It may have meant “Look out!” yet it seemed
to me like the asking of alms: “Here we are, three hungry neighbors of yours;
give us food.” So I brought out the entrails and legs of a chicken, and placed
them upon the snow. The crows very soon discovered what I had done, and with
the usual suspicious movement of the closed wings which has the effect of
emphasizing the birds’ alertness, approached and devoured the food or carried
it away. But there was not the least strife or dispute among them over the
food. Indeed, each seemed ready to give precedence to the others. In fact, the
crow is a courtly, fine-mannered bird. Birds of prey, will rend one another
over their food; even buzzards will make some show of mauling one another with
their wings; but I have yet to see anything of the kind with that gentle
freebooter, the crow. Yet suspicion is his dominant trait. Anything that looks
like design puts him on his guard. The simplest device in a cornfield usually
suffices to keep him away. He suspects a trap. His wit is not deep, but it is
quick, and ever on the alert.
One of our natural
history romancers makes the crows flock in June. But the truth is, they do not
flock till September. Through the summer the different families keep pretty
well together. You may see the old ones with their young foraging about the
fields, the young often being fed by their parents.
From my boyhood I
have seen the yearly meeting of the crows in September or October, on a high
grassy hill or a wooded ridge. Apparently, all the crows from a large area
assemble at these times; you may see them coming, singly or in loose bands,
from all directions to the rendezvous, till there are hundreds of them
together. They make black an acre or two of ground. At intervals they all rise
in the air, and wheel about, all cawing at once. Then to the ground again, or
to the tree-tops, as the case may be; then, rising again, they send forth the
voice of the multitude. What does it all mean? I notice that this rally is
always preliminary to their going into winter quarters. It would be interesting
to know just the nature of the communication that takes place between them. Not
long afterwards, or early in October, they may be seen morning and evening
going to and from their rookeries. The matter seems to be settled in these
September gatherings of the clan. was the spot agreed upon beforehand and
notice served upon all the members of the tribe? Our “school-of-the-woods”
professors would probably infer something of the kind. I suspect it is all
brought about as naturally as any other aggregation of animals. A few crows
meet on the hill; they attract others and still others. The rising of a body of
them in the air, the circling and cawing, may be an instinctive act to
advertise the meeting to all the crows within sight or hearing. At any rate,
it has this effect, and they come hurrying from all points.
What their various
calls mean, who shall tell? That lusty caw-aw,
caw-aw that one hears in spring and summer, like the voice of
authority or command, what does it mean? I never could find out. It is
doubtless from the male. A crow will utter it while sitting alone on the fence
in the pasture, as well as when flying through the air. The crow’s cry of alarm
is easily distinguished; all the other birds and wild creatures know it, and
the hunter who is stalking his game is apt to swear when he hears it. I have
heard two crows in the spring, seated on a limb close together, give utterance
to many curious, guttural, gurgling, ventriloquial sounds. What were they
saying? It was probably some form of the language of love.
I venture to say
that no one has ever yet heard the crow utter a complaining or a disconsolate
note. He is always cheery, he is always self-possessed, he is a great success.
Nothing in Bermuda made me feel so much at home as a flock of half a dozen of
our crows which I saw and heard there. At one time they were very numerous on
the island, but they have been persecuted till only a remnant of the tribe
remains.
I
My friend and
neighbor through the year,
Self-appointed
overseer
Of my crops of
fruit and grain,
Of my woods and
furrowed plain,
Claim thy tithings
right and left,
I shall never call
it theft.
Nature wisely made
the law,
And I fail to find
a flaw
In thy title to the
earth,
And all it holds of
any worth.
I like thy
self-complacent air,
I like thy ways so
free from care,
Thy landlord stroll
about my fields,
Quickly noting what
each yields;
Thy courtly mien
and bearing bold,
As if thy claim
were bought with gold;
Thy floating shape
against the sky,
When days are calm
and clouds sail high;
Thy thrifty flight
ere rise of sun,
Thy homing clans
when day is done.
Hues protective are
not thine,
So sleek thy coat
each quill doth shine.
Diamond black to
end of toe,
Thy counter-point
the crystal snow.
II
Never plaintive nor
appealing,
Quite at home when
thou art stealing,
Always groomed to
tip of feather,
Calm and trim in
every weather,
Morn till night my
woods policing,
Every sound thy
watch increasing.
Hawk and owl in
tree-top hiding
Feel the shame of
thy deriding.
Naught escapes thy
observation,
None but dread thy
accusation.
Hunters, prowlers,
woodland lovers
Vainly seek the
leafy covers.
III
Noisy, scheming,
and predacious,
With demeanor
almost gracious,
Dowered with
leisure, void of hurry,
Void of fuss and
void of worry,
Friendly bandit,
Robin Hood,
Judge and jury of
the wood,
Or Captain Kidd of
sable quill,
Hiding treasures in
the hill,
Nature made thee
for each season,
Gave thee wit for
ample reason,
Good crow wit that
‘s always burnished
Like the coat her
care has furnished.
May thy numbers
ne’er diminish,
I’ll befriend thee
till life’s finish.
May I never cease
to meet thee,
May I never have to
eat thee.
And mayest thou
never have to fare so
That thou playest the part of scarecrow. |
|